372
Views
23
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Fine motor skills enhance lexical processing of embodied vocabulary: A test of the nimble-hands, nimble-minds hypothesis

&
Pages 2169-2187 | Received 08 Jun 2016, Accepted 16 Aug 2016, Published online: 14 Sep 2016

References

  • Amsel, B. D., & Cree, G. S. (2013). Semantic richness, concreteness, and object domain: An electrophysiological study. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 67, 117–129. doi:10.1037/a0029807
  • Anderson, M. L. (2007a). Evolution of cognitive function via redeployment of brain areas. The Neuroscientist, 13, 13–21. doi: 10.1177/1073858406294706
  • Anderson, M. L. (2007b). The massive redeployment hypothesis and the functional topography of the brain. Philosophical Psychology, 20, 143–174. doi:10.1080/09515080701197163
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716–724. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x
  • Bart, O., Hajami, D., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2007). Predicting school adjustment from motor abilities in kindergarten. Infant and Child Development, 16, 597–615. doi:10.1002/icd.514
  • Becker, D. R., Miao, A., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2014). Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 411–424. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.014
  • Beilock, S. L., Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., Nusbaum, H. C., & Small, S. L. (2008). Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 13269–13273. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803424105
  • Bennett, S. D. R., Burnett, A. N., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2011). Imageability and body-object interaction ratings for 599 multisyllabic nouns. Behavior research methods, 43(4), 1100–1109. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0117-5
  • Birchenough, J. M. H., Davies, R., & Connelly, V. (2016). Rated age-of-acquisition norms for over 3,200 German words. Behavior Research Methods. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0718-0
  • Brookman, A., McDonald, S., McDonald, D., & Bishop, D. V. (2013). Fine motor deficits in Reading disability and language impairment: Same or different? PeerJ, 1(3), e217. doi:10.7717/peerj.217
  • Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D., & Morrison, F. J. (2012). Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Development, 83, 1229–1244. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01768.x
  • Connell, C., & Lynott, D. (2016). Embodied semantic effects in visual word recognition. In M. H. Fischer & Y. Coello (Eds.), Conceptual and interactive embodiment (pp. 71–92). London, NewYork, NY: Routledge.
  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125(3), 452–465. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.010
  • Davis, E. E., Pitchford, N. J., & Limback, E. (2011). The interrelation between cognitive and motor development in typically developing children aged 4–11 years is underpinned by visual processing and fine manual control. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 569–584. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02018.x
  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, 384–398. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004
  • Dellatolas, G., De Agostini, M., Curt, F., Kremin, H., Letierce, A., Maccario, J., & Lellouch, J. (2003). Manual skill, hand skill asymmetry, and cognitive performances in young children. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain, and Cognition, 8, 317–338. doi:10.1080/1357650
  • Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117
  • Dinehart, L., & Manfra, L. (2013). Associations between low-income children’s fine motor skills in preschool and academic performance in second grade. Early Education & Development, 24, 138–161. doi:10.1080/10409289.2011.636729
  • Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., & Carreiras, M. (2008). Noa’s ark: Influence of the number of associates in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1072–1077. doi:10.3758/pbr.15.6.1072
  • Fischer, M. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: A review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 825–850. doi:10.1080/17470210701623605
  • Gabbard, C., Caçola, P., & Rodrigues, L. P. (2008). A New inventory for assessing Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development (AHEMD-SR). Early Childhood Education Journal, 36, 5–9. doi:10.1007/s10643-008-0235-6
  • Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1008–1017. doi: 10.1037/a0020104
  • Hallgren, K. A. (2012). Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: An overview and tutorial. Tutor Quant Methods Psychology, 8, 23–34. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.08.1.p023
  • Hargreaves, I. S., Leonard, G. A., Pexman, P. M., Pittman, D. J., Siakaluk, P. D., & Goodyear, B. G. (2012). The neural correlates of the body-object interaction effect in semantic processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00022
  • Hargreaves, I. S., Pexman, P. M., Johnson, J. C., & Zdrazilova, L. (2012). Richer concepts are better remembered: Number of features effects in free recall. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00073
  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, Reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491–528. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.491
  • Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 149–171. doi:10.1080/13682820010019874
  • Hills, T. T., Maouene, M., Maouene, J., Sheya, A., & Smith, L. (2009). Longitudinal analysis of early semantic networks: Preferential attachment or preferential acquisition? Psychological Science, 20, 729–739. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02365.x
  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1968). The growth of logical thinking. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd.
  • Inkster, M., Wellsby, M., Lloyd, E., & Pexman, P. M. (2016). Development of embodied word meanings: sensorimotor effects in children’s lexical processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(506), 1. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00317
  • Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 229–261. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990432
  • James, K. H., & Gauthier, I. (2006). Letter processing automatically recruits a sensory–motor brain network. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2937–2949. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.026
  • James, K. H., & Maouene, J. (2009). Auditory verb perception recruits motor systems in the developing brain: An fMRI investigation. Developmental Science, 12, F26–F34. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00919.x
  • Juhasz, B. J., Yap, M. J., Dicke, J., Taylor, S. C., & Gullick, M. M. (2011). Tangible words are recognized faster: The grounding of meaning in sensory and perceptual systems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1683–1691. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.605150
  • Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. E., & Chidekel, D. (2012). From movement to thought: Executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. The Cerebellum, 11, 505–525. doi:10.1007/s12311-011-0321-y
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2010). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought ([Nachdr.]). New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2007). Answers to 20 questions about interrater reliability and interrater agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 11, 815–852. doi:10.1177/1094428106296642
  • Lenhard, A., Lenhard, W., Segerer, R., & Suggate, S. (2015). Peabody picture vocabulary test – 4. Ausgabe: Deutsche Fassung. Frankfurt am Main: Pearson Assessment.
  • Levine, S. C., Ratliff, K. R., Huttenlocher, J., & Cannon, J. (2012). Early puzzle play: A predictor of preschoolers’ spatial transformation skill. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 530–542. doi:10.1037/a0025913
  • Luo, Z., Jose, P. E., Huntsinger, C. S., & Pigott, T. D. (2007). Fine motor skills and mathematics achievement in East Asian American and European American kindergartners and first graders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 595–614. doi:10.1348/026151007X185329
  • Maouene, J., Hidaka, S., & Smith, L. (2008). Body parts and early-learned verbs. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 32, 1200–1216. doi:10.1080/03640210802019997
  • Maouene, J., Sethuraman, N., Laakso, A., & Maouene, M. (2011). The body region correlates of concrete and abstract verbs in early child language. Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 449–484.
  • Martzog, P. (2015). Feinmotorische Fertigkeiten und kognitive Fähigkeiten bei Kindern im Vorschulalter [Fine motor skills and cogntive development in preschool children] (1. Ed.). Marburg: Tectum.
  • McPhillips, M., & Jordan-Black, J. (2007). The effect of social disadvantage on motor development in young children: A comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1214–1222. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01814.x
  • Morrison, F. J., & Hindman, A. H. (2012). Promoting school readiness: An integrative framework. In S. P. Suggate & E. Reese (Eds.), Contemporary debates in child development and education (pp. 133–142). London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
  • Nelson, K. E., Welsh, J. A., Trup, E. M. V., & Greenberg, M. T. (2011). Language delays of impoverished preschool children in relation to early academic and emotion recognition skills. First Language, 31, 164–194. doi:10.1177/0142723710391887
  • Pagani, L. S., Fitzpatrick, C., Archambault, I., & Janosz, M. (2010). School readiness and later achievement: A French Canadian replication and extension. Developmental Psychology, 46, 984–994. doi: 10.1037/a0018881
  • Petermann, F., Bös, K., & Kastner, J. (2011). Movement assessment battery for children - second edition. Frankfurt am Main: Pearson Assessment.
  • Petermann, F., & Lipsius, M. (2009). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence 3rd edition-Deutschsprachige Adaptation nach D. Wechlser. Frankfurt am Main: Pearson Assessment.
  • Roebers, C. M., Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., & Jäger, K. (2014). The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children’s transition to school: A latent variable approach. Human Movement Science, 33, 284–297. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.011
  • Sajin, S. M., & Connine, C. M. (2014). Semantic richness: The role of semantic features in processing spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 13–35. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.006
  • Schroeder, S., Würzner, K.-M., Heister, J., Geyken, A., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Childlex–Eine lexikalische Datenbank zur Schriftsprache für Kinder im Deutschen. Psychologische Rundschau, 66, 155–165. doi:10.1026/0033-3042/a000275
  • Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future Reading. In D. K. Dickinson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy (pp. 173–182). New York & London: Guilford Press.
  • Shapiro, L. (2011). Embodied cognition. Oxon: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
  • Siakaluk, P., Pexman, P., Sears, C., Wilson, K., Locheed, K., & Owen, W. (2008). The benefits of sensorimotor knowledge: Body-object interaction facilitates semantic processing. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 32, 591–605. doi:10.1080/03640210802035399
  • Siakaluk, P. D., Pexman, P. M., Aguilera, L., Owen, W. J., & Sears, C. R. (2008). Evidence for the activation of sensorimotor information during visual word recognition: The body–object interaction effect. Cognition, 106(1), 433–443. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.011
  • Sidhu, D. M., Kwan, R., Pexman, P. M., & Siakaluk, P. D. (2014). Effects of relative embodiment in lexical and semantic processing of verbs. Acta Psychologica, 149, 32–39. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.009
  • Smirni, P., & Zappala, G. (1989). Manual behavior, lateralization of manual skills and cognitive performance of preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 267–272. doi: 10.2466/pms.1989.68.1.267
  • Soden-Fraunhofen, R. von, Sim, E., Liebich, S., Frank, K., & Kiefer, M. (2008). Die Rolle der motorischen Interaktion beim Erwerb begrifflichen Wissens: Eine Trainingsstudie mit künstlichen Objekten [The role of motor interactions during the acquisition of conceptual knowledge: A traing study with novel objects]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 22, 47–58. doi:10.1024/1010-0652.22.1.47
  • Son, S.-H., & Meisels, S. J. (2006). The relationship of young children’s motor skills to later school achievement. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 755–778. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0033
  • Suggate, S. P., Pufke, E., & Stoeger, H. (2016). The effect of fine and grapho-motor skill demands on preschoolers’ decoding skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 34–48. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.012
  • Suggate, S. P., Reese, E., Lenhard, W., & Schneider, W. (2014). The relative contributions of vocabulary, decoding, and phonemic awareness to word reading in English versus German. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1395–1412. doi:10.1007/s11145-014-9498-z
  • Suggate, S. P., & Stoeger, H. (2014). Do nimble hands make for nimble lexicons? Fine motor skills predict knowledge of embodied vocabulary items. First Language, 34, 244–261. doi:10.1177/0142723714535768
  • Suggate, S. P., Stoeger, H., & Pufke, E. (2016). Relations between playing activities and fine motor skills. Early Child Development and Care. doi:10.1080/03004430.2016.1167047
  • Thelen, E. (2000). Grounded in the world: Developmental origins of the embodied mind. Infancy, 1, 3–28. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0101_02
  • Tramontana, M. G., Hooper, S. R., & Selzer, S. C. (1988). Research on the preschool prediction of later academic achievement: A review. Developmental Review, 8, 89–146. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(88)90001-9
  • Vacc, N. A., Vacc, N. N., & Fogleman, M. S. (1987). Preschool screening: Using the DIAL as a predictor of first-grade performance. Journal of School Psychology, 25, 45–51. doi:10.1016/0022-4405(87)90059-8
  • van der Fels, Irene M. J., te Wierike, S. C., Hartman, E., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Smith, J., & Visscher, C. (2015). The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16 year old typically developing children: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 18(6), 697–703. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.007
  • Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2005). Der Zusammenhang zwischen motorischer und kognitiver Entwicklung im frühen Kindesalter- ein Teilergebnis der MODALIS-Studie [The relationship between motoric and cognitive development in early childhood – A partial result from the MODALIS Study]. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Sportmedizin, 56, 358–359.
  • Wassenberg, R., Feron, F. J. M., Kessels, A. G. H., Hendriksen, J. G. M., Kalff, A. C., Kroes, … Vles, J. S. H. (2005). Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: Results from a large-scale cross-sectional study. Child Development, 76, 1092–1103. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00899.x
  • Wellsby, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2014a). Developing embodied cognition: Insights from children’s concepts and language processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 551. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00506
  • Wellsby, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2014b). The influence of bodily experience on children’s language processing. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 425–441. doi:10.1111/tops.12092
  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/BF03196322
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 1–11. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.