47,323
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

‘Unexpected’ spelling difficulty in a 10-year-old child with good reading skills: An intervention case study

&
Pages 143-166 | Received 22 Sep 2015, Accepted 22 Feb 2016, Published online: 23 May 2016

References

  • Aliminosa, D., McCloskey, M., Goodman-Shulman, R., & Sokol, S. M. (1993). Remediation of acquired dysgraphia as a technique for testing interpretations of deficits. Aphasiology, 7, 55–69. doi: 10.1080/02687039308249499
  • Angelelli, P., Marinelli, C. V., & Zoccolotti, P. (2010). Single or dual orthographic representations for reading and spelling? A study of Italian dyslexic–dysgraphic and normal children. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27, 305–333. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2010.543539
  • Barry, C. (1994). Spelling routes (or Roots or Rutes). In G. D. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 27–49). John Wiley & Sons Ltd: Chichester, England.
  • Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009
  • Bowey, J. A., & Rutherford, J. (2007). Imbalanced word-reading profiles in eighth-graders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 169–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.11.001
  • Broom, Y. M., & Doctor, E. A. (1995a). Developmental surface dyslexia: A case study of the efficacy of a remediation programme. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 69–110. doi: 10.1080/02643299508251992
  • Broom, Y. M., & Doctor, E. A. (1995b). Developmental phonological dyslexia: A case study of the efficacy of a remediation programme. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 725–766. doi: 10.1080/02643299508251400
  • Bruck, M., & Waters, G. (1990). Effects of reading skill on component spelling skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 425–437. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400009668
  • Brunsdon, R., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2005). Treatment of irregular word spelling in developmental surface dysgraphia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 213–251. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000077
  • Brunsdon, R. K., Hannan, T. J., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2002). Treatment of lexical processing in mixed dyslexia: A case study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 385–418. doi: 10.1080/09602010244000174
  • Burden, V. (1992). Why are some “normal” readers such poor spellers? In C. M. Sterling & C. Robson (Eds.), Psychology, spelling and education (pp. 200–214). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
  • Caramazza, A., & Miceli, G. (1990). The structure of graphemic representations. Cognition, 37, 243–297. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90047-N
  • Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Villa, G., & Romani, C. (1987). The role of the graphemic buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 26, 59–85. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90014-X
  • Coltheart, M., & Leahy, J. (1996). Assessment of lexical and nonlexical reading abilities in children: Some normative data. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 136–140. doi: 10.1080/00049539608259520
  • Crawford, J. R., & Howell, D. C. (1998). Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section D) 12, 482–486. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.4.482.7241
  • Dubois, M., Kyllingsbæk, S., Prado, C., Musca, S. C., Peiffer, E., Lassus-Sangosse, D., & Valdois, S. (2010). Fractionating the multi-character processing deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Cortex, 46, 717–738. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.002
  • Dunn, L., Dunn, M. & Whetton, C. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (BPVS) (2nd ed.) London: GL Assessment.
  • Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., … Zorzi, M. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21232
  • Frederickson, N., Frith, U., & Reason, R. (1997). Phonological assessment battery. London: GL Assessment.
  • Forum for Research in Language and Literacy Group. (2012). Diagnostic test of word reading processes. London: GL Assessment.
  • Frith, U. (1978). From print to meaning and from print to sound, or how to read without knowing how to spell. Visible Language, 12, 43–54.
  • Frith, U. (1980). Unexpected spelling problems. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 495–515). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface Dyslexia, Neuropsychological and Cognitive Studies of Phonological Reading (pp. 301–330). London: Erlbaum.
  • Goff, D.A., Pratt, C., & Ong, B. (2005). The relations between children’s reading comprehension, working memory, language skills, and components of reading decoding in a normal sample. Reading and Writing, 18, 583–616. doi: 10.1007/s11145-004-7109-0
  • Goulandris, N. K., & Snowling, M. (1991). Visual memory deficits: A plausible cause of developmental dyslexia? Evidence from a single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 127–154. doi: 10.1080/02643299108253369
  • Hanley, J. R., & Kay, J. (1998). Note: Does the graphemic buffer play a role in reading? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 313–318. doi: 10.1080/026432998381195
  • Hanley, J. R., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: An orthographic processing impairment. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 44, 285–319. doi: 10.1080/02724989243000046
  • Hillis, A.E., & Caramazza, A. (1989). The graphemic buffer and attentional mechanisms. Brain and Language, 36, 208–235. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(89)90062-X
  • Holmes, V., & Castles, A. (2001). Unexpectedly poor spelling in university students. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 319–350. doi: 10.1207/S1532799XSSR0504_02
  • Holmes, V. M., Malone, A. M., & Redenbach, H. (2008). Orthographic processing and visual sequential memory in unexpectedly poor spellers. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 136–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00364.x
  • Holmes, V. M., & Quinn, L. (2009). Unexpectedly poor spelling and phonological-processing skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13 (4), 295–317. doi: 10.1080/10888430903001225
  • Hulme, C. (1981). Reading retardation and multi-sensory teaching. London: Routledge.
  • Judica, A., Luca, M. D., & Spinelli, D. (2002). Training of developmental surface dyslexia improves reading performance and shortens eye fixation duration in reading. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 177–197. doi: 10.1080/09602010244000002
  • Kohnen, S., Nickels, L., Brunsdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Patterns of generalisation after treating sub-lexical spelling deficits in a child with mixed dysgraphia. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 157–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00366.x
  • Kohnen, S., Nickels, L., & Coltheart, M. (2010). Training “rule-of-〈E〉”: Further investigation of a previously successful intervention for a spelling rule in developmental mixed dysgraphia. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 392–413. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01425.x
  • Kohnen, S., Nickels, L., Coltheart, M., & Brunsdon, R. (2008). Predicting generalization in the training of irregular-word spelling: Treating lexical spelling deficits in a child. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 343–375. doi: 10.1080/02643290802003000
  • Masterson, J., Colombo, L., Spencer, K., Ftika, K., Quinn, L., & Syntili, A. (2008). Factors affecting the spelling performance of English-, Greek- and Italian-speaking children in grade 1/2. Paper presented at Writing Systems: Analysis, acquisition and use. Institute of Education, University of London.
  • Masterson, J., Laxon, V., Lovejoy, S., & Morris, V. (2007). Phonological skill, lexical decision and letter report performance in good and poor adult spellers. Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 429–442. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00347.x
  • Masterson, J., Stuart, M., Dixon, M., & Lovejoy, S. (2010). Children's printed word database: Continuities and changes over time in children's early reading vocabulary. British Journal of Psychology, 101(2), 221–242.
  • McCloskey, M., Macaruso, P., & Rapp, B. (2006). Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 278–307. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000518
  • Miceli, G., & Capasso, R. (2006). Spelling and dysgraphia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 110–134. doi: 10.1080/02643290500202730
  • Mitchum, C., & Berndt, R. S. (1995). The cognitive neuropsychological approach to treatment of language disorders. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 5, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/09602019508520173
  • Naglieri, J. A. (1985). Matrix analogies test short form. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.
  • Niolaki, G. Z., & Masterson, J. (2013). Intervention for a multi-character processing deficit in a Greek-speaking child with surface dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 208–232. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2013.842892
  • Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–566. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554
  • Paraskevopoulos, I. N., Kalantzi-Azizi, A., & Giannitsas, N. D. (1999). Αθηνά τεστ διάγνωσης δυσκολιών μάθησης – Athena test: Diagnosis of learning difficulties. Athens: Ellinika Grammata.
  • de Partz, M.-P. (1995). Deficit of the graphemic buffer: Effects of a written lexical segmentation strategy. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 5, 129–147. doi: 10.1080/09602019508520178
  • de Partz, M.-P., Seron, X., & Linden, M. V. D. (1992). Re-education of a surface dysgraphia with a visual imagery strategy. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 369–401. doi: 10.1080/02643299208252065
  • Perin, D. (1983). Phonemic segmentation and spelling. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 129–144. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1983.tb01849.x
  • Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 235–257. doi: 10.1080/10888430701344306
  • Rapcsak, S. Z., Henry, M. L., Teague, S. L., Carnahan, S. D., & Beeson, P. M. (2007). Do dual-route models accurately predict reading and spelling performance in individuals with acquired alexia and agraphia? Neuropsychologia, 45, 2519–2524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.019
  • Rapp, B. (2005). The relationship between treatment outcomes and the underlying cognitive deficit: Evidence from the remediation of acquired dysgraphia. Aphasiology, 19, 994–1008. doi: 10.1080/02687030544000209
  • Rapp, B., & Kane, A. (2002). Remediation of deficits affecting different components of the spelling process. Aphasiology, 16, 439–454. doi: 10.1080/02687030244000301
  • Romani, C., Ward, J., & Olson, A. (1999). Developmental surface dysgraphia: What is the underlying cognitive impairment? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52, 97–128. doi: 10.1080/713755804
  • Rowse, H. J., & Wilshire, C. E. (2007). Comparison of phonological and whole-word treatments for two contrasting cases of developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 817–842. doi: 10.1080/02643290701764207
  • Rudra, A. (2005). Cognitive correlates of poor and advanced reading abilities in children. Unpublished PhD Thesis, University of Essex.
  • Rust, J., & Golombok, S. (2006). Wechsler individual achievement test second UK edition for teachers. London: Harcourt Assessment.
  • Sacre, L., & Masterson, J. (2000). Single word spelling test. London: NfER-Nelson.
  • Sage, K., & Ellis, A. (2006). Using orthographic neighbours to treat a case of graphemic buffer disorder. Aphasiology, 20, 851–870. doi: 10.1080/02687030600738945
  • Schiller, N. O., Greenhall, J. A., Shelton, J. R., & Caramazza, A. (2001). Serial order effects in spelling errors: Evidence from two dysgraphic patients. Neurocase, 7, 1–14. doi: 10.1093/neucas/7.1.1
  • Spencer, K. (1999). Predicting word-spelling difficulty in 7-to 11-year-olds. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 283–292. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.00091
  • Stadie, N., & van de Vijver, R. (2003). A linguistic and neuropsychological approach to remediation in a German case of developmental dysgraphia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 280–299. doi: 10.1007/s11881-003-0013-5
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360–407. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1
  • Tainturier, M. J., & Rapp, B. (2003). Is a single graphemic buffer used in reading and spelling? Aphasiology, 17, 537–562. doi: 10.1080/02687030344000021
  • Tainturier, M. J., & Rapp, B. (2001). The spelling process. In B. Rapp (Ed.), What deficits reveal about the human mind/brain: A handbook of cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 1–27). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
  • Valdois, S., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Reilhac, C., N'guyen-Morel, M.-A., Guinet, E., & Orliaguet, J.-P. (2011). A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex, 47, 1197–1218. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.011
  • Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., Zorman, M., David, D., & Pellat, J. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Reading and Writing, 16, 541–572. doi: 10.1023/A:1025501406971
  • Venezky, R. (1970). The structure of English orthography. Walter de Gruyter: Berlin.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin: TX: PRO-ED.
  • Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children fourth UK edition. Pearson Publishers: London.
  • Weekes, B., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 277–315. doi: 10.1080/026432996382033
  • Yachini, M., & Friedmann, N. (2010). Developmental graphemic buffer dysgraphia. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 6, 148–149. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.073