21,160
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Theory of mind and children’s understanding of teaching and learning during early childhood

| (Reviewing Editor)
Article: 1011973 | Received 09 Jun 2014, Accepted 22 Jan 2015, Published online: 19 Feb 2015

References

  • Apperly, I. (2012). What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 825–839.10.1080/17470218.2012.676055
  • Astington, J. W. (1998). Theory of mind goes to school. Educational Leadership, 56, 46–48.
  • Astington, J. W., & Pelletier, J. (2005). Theory of mind, language, and learning in the early years: Developmental originals of school readiness. In B. D. Homer & C. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), The development of social cognition and communication (pp. 205–230). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Bereiter, C. (2002). Education and mind in the knowledge age. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Bernstein, D. M., Atance, C. M., Meltzoff, A. N., & Loftus, G. R. (2007). Hindsight bias and developing theories of mind. Child Development, 78, 1374–1394.10.1111/cdev.2007.78.issue-4
  • Birch, S. A. J. (2005). When knowledge is a curse. Children’s and adults’ reasoning about mental states. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 25–29.10.1111/cdir.2005.14.issue-1
  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Bering, J. M. (2002). The evolved child: Applying evolutionary developmental psychology to modern schooling. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 347–373.
  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663.10.1111/cdev.2007.78.issue-2
  • Burr, J. E., & Hofer, B. K. (2002). Personal epistemology and theory of mind: Deciphering young children’s beliefs about knowledge and knowing. New Ideas in Psychology, 20, 199–224.10.1016/S0732-118X(02)00010-7
  • Carruthers, P. (2009). How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 121–138.10.1017/S0140525X09000545
  • Davis-Unger, A. C., & Carlson, S. M. (2008). Development of teaching skills and relations to theory of mind in preschoolers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9, 26–45.10.1080/15248370701836584
  • Esbensen, B. M., Taylor, M., & Stoess, C. (1997). Children’s behavioral understanding of knowledge acquisition. Cognitive Development, 12, 53–84.10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90030-7
  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
  • Flavell, J. H., & Miller, P. H. (1998). Social cognition. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R. Sigler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed., pp. 851–898). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Frye, D., & Wang, Z. (2008). Theory of mind, understanding teaching, and early childhood education. In S. K. Thurman & C. A. Fiorello (Eds.), Applied cognitive research in K-3 classrooms (pp. 85–108). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Frye, D., & Ziv, M. (2005). Teaching and learning as intentional activities. In B. D. Homer & C. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), The development of social cognition and communication (pp. 231–258). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.10.1037/10871-000
  • Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26–37.10.2307/1130386
  • Gopnik, A., & Graf, P. (1988). Knowing how you know: Young children’s ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs. Child Development, 59, 1366–1371.10.2307/1130499
  • Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085–1108.10.1037/a0028044
  • Harris, P. L. (2002). What do children learn from testimony? In P. Carruthers, S. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 316–334). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511613517
  • Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: From toddlerhood through to the transition to school. East Sussex: Psychology Press.
  • Koenig, M. A. (2012). Beyond semantic accuracy: Preschoolers evaluate a speaker’s reasons. Child Development, 83, 1051–1063.10.1111/cdev.2012.83.issue-3
  • Kuhn, D. (2000). Theory of mind, metacognition, and reasoning: A life-span perspective. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 301–326). Hove: Psychological Press.
  • Kuhn, D., & Weinstock, M. (2002). What is epistemological thinking and why does it matter? In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 121–144). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Lang, B., & Perner, J. (2002). Understanding of intention and false belief and the development of self-control. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 67–76.10.1348/026151002166325
  • Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2007). Knowledge about the mind: Links between theory of mind and later metamemory. Child Development, 78, 148–167.10.1111/cdev.2007.78.issue-1
  • Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (2002). Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. Child Development, 73, 1073–1084.10.1111/cdev.2002.73.issue-4
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10, 126–134.10.1111/desc.2007.10.issue-1
  • Mills, C. M. (2013). Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology, 49, 404–418.10.1037/a0029500
  • Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Knowing the limits of one’s understanding: The development of an awareness of an illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(1), 1–32.10.1016/j.jecp.2003.09.003
  • Montgomery, D. E., & Lightner, M. (2004). Children’s developing understanding of differences between their own intentional action and passive movement. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 417–438.10.1348/0261510041552701
  • Naito, M. (2003). The relationship between theory of mind and episodic memory: Evidence for the development of autonoetic consciousness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 312–336.10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00075-4
  • Nurmsoo, E., & Robinson, E. J. (2009). Children’s trust in previously inaccurate informants who were well or poorly informed: When past errors can be excused. Child Development, 80, 23–27.10.1111/cdev.2009.80.issue-1
  • Olson, D. R., & Katz, S. (2001). The fourth folk pedagogy. In B. Torff & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Understanding and teaching the intuitive mind: Student and teacher learning (pp. 243–263). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  • Pillow, B. H. (1999). Children’s understanding of inferential knowledge. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 160, 419–428.10.1080/00221329909595555
  • Povinelli, D. J., & DeBlois, S. (1992). Young children’s (Homo sapiens) understanding of knowledge formation in themselves and others. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 228–238.10.1037/0735-7036.106.3.228
  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.10.1017/S0140525X00076512
  • Robinson, E. J., & Nurmsoo, E. (2009). When do children learn from unreliable speakers? Cognitive Development, 24, 16–22.10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.08.001
  • Rosset, E. (2008). It’s no accident: Our bias for intentional explanations. Cognition, 108, 771–780.10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.001
  • Royzman, E. B., Cassidy, K. W., & Baron, J. (2003). “I know, you know”: Epistemic egocentrism in children and adults. Review of General Psychology, 7, 38–65.10.1037/1089-2680.7.1.38
  • Russell, J., Hill, E. L., & Franco, F. (2001). The role of belief veracity in understanding intentions-in-action. Cognitive Development, 16, 775–792.10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00057-0
  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070.10.1111/cdev.2001.72.issue-4
  • Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 417–457.10.1017/S0140525X00005756
  • Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139173452
  • Sobel, D. M., Li, J., & Corriveau, K. H. (2007). “They danced around in my head and I learned them”: Children’s developing conceptions of learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 345–369.10.1080/15248370701446806
  • Sodian, B. (1988). Children’s attributions of knowledge to the listener in a referential communication task. Child Development, 59, 378–385.10.2307/1130317
  • Sperling, R. A., Walls, R. T., & Hill, L. A. (2000). Early relationships among self-regulatory constructs: Theory of mind and preschool children’s problem solving. Child Study Journal, 30, 233–252.
  • Taylor, M., Esbensen, B. M., & Bennett, R. T. (1994). Children’s understanding of knowledge acquisition: The tendency for children to report that they have always known what they have just learned. Child Development, 65, 1581–1604.10.2307/1131282
  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–511.10.1017/S0140525X0003123X
  • Wang, Z. (2010a). Children’s picture book and theory of mind development: From McDull to the true story of the three little pigs. Hong Kong Journal of Early Childhood, 9, 44–51.
  • Wang, Z. (2010b). Mindful learning: Children’s developing theory of mind and their understanding of the concept of learning ( Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.
  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.10.1111/cdev.2001.72.issue-3
  • Wellman, H. M., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2004). Theory of mind for learning and teaching: The nature and role of explanation. Cognitive Development, 19, 479–497.10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.09.003
  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  • Whitebread, D. (2013). Too much, too young. New Scientist, 220, 28–29.10.1016/S0262-4079(13)62688-3
  • Wood, D. (1998). How children think and learn (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Boseovski, J. J. (2001). Video reminders in a representational change task: Memory for cues but not beliefs or statements. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 107–129.10.1006/jecp.2000.2562
  • Ziv, M., & Frye, D. (2004). Children’s understanding of teaching: The role of knowledge and belief. Cognitive Development, 19, 457–477.10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.09.002
  • Ziv, M., Solomon, A., & Frye, D. (2008). Young children’s recognition of the intentionality of teaching. Child Development, 79, 1237–1256.10.1111/cdev.2008.79.issue-5