1,745
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLE

Word learning in the context of semantic prior knowledge: evidence of interference from feature-based neighbours in children and adults

ORCID Icon, , , &
Pages 157-174 | Received 08 Apr 2022, Accepted 07 Jul 2022, Published online: 02 Nov 2022

References

  • Antony, J. W., Ferreira, C. S., Norman, K. A., & Wimber, M. (2017). Retrieval as a fast route to memory consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(8), 573–576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.05.001
  • Ashworth, A., Hill, C. M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Dimitriou, D. (2014). Sleep enhances memory consolidation in children. Journal of Sleep Research, 23(3), 304–310. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12119
  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (release 2). Distributed by the Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H. (2015a). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv Preprint ArXiv, 1506, 04967. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1506.04967
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015b). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Bishop, D. V., Barry, J. G., & Hardiman, M. J. (2012). Delayed retention of new word-forms is better in children than adults regardless of language ability: A factorial two-way study. PloS one, 7(5), e37326. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0037326
  • Borovsky, A., Ellis, E. M., Evans, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (2016). Lexical leverage: Category knowledge boosts real-time novel word recognition in 2-year-olds. Developmental Science, 19(6), 918–932. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12343
  • Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5
  • Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C. (2001). Characterizing semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 531–544. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196189
  • Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). Ordinal-Regression models for ordinal data. R Package Version 2015.6-28. http://www.cran.r-project.org/package=ordinal.
  • Clay, F., Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., & Hanley, D. A. (2007). Teaching adults new words: The role of practice and consolidation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(5), 970. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.970
  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 33(4), 497–505. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748108400805
  • Davies, R. A., Arnell, R., Birchenough, J. M., Grimmond, D., & Houlson, S. (2017). Reading through the life span: Individual differences in psycholinguistic effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(8), 1298. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000366
  • Davis, M. H., & Gaskell, M. G. (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3773–3800. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0111
  • Dumay, N., & Gaskell, M. G. (2007). Sleep-associated changes in the mental representation of spoken words. Psychological Science, 18(1), 35–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x
  • Engelthaler, T., & Hills, T. T. (2017). Feature biases in early word learning: Network distinctiveness predicts age of acquisition. Cognitive Science, 41, 120–140. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12350
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). Dmdx: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116–124. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503
  • Gais, S., Lucas, B., & Born, J. (2006). Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learning & Memory, 13(3), 259–262. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.132106
  • Gaskell, M. G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1997). Integrating form and meaning: A distributed model of speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5-6), 613–656. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909697386646
  • Goossens, N. A., Camp, G., Verkoeijen, P. P., & Tabbers, H. K. (2014). The effect of retrieval practice in primary school vocabulary learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(1), 135–142. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2956
  • Grondin, R., Lupker, S. J., & McRae, K. (2009). Shared features dominate semantic richness effects for concrete concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.09.001
  • Hameau, S., Nickels, L., & Biedermann, B. (2019). Effects of semantic neighbourhood density on spoken word production. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(12), 2752–2775. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021819859850
  • Henderson, L. M., Weighall, A., & Gaskell, G. (2013). Learning new vocabulary during childhood: Effects of semantic training on lexical consolidation and integration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(3), 572–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.004
  • Henderson, L. M., Weighall, A. R., Brown, H., & Gaskell, M. G. (2012). Consolidation of vocabulary is associated with sleep in children. Developmental Science, 15(5), 674–687. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01172.x
  • Hills, T. T., Maouene, M., Maouene, J., Sheya, A., & Smith, L. (2009). Longitudinal analysis of early semantic networks. Psychological Science, 20(6), 729–739. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02365.x
  • Hoover, J. R., Storkel, H. L., & Hogan, T. P. (2010). A cross-sectional comparison of the effects of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on word learning by preschool children. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(1), 100–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.02.003
  • Hulme, R. C., & Rodd, J. M. (2021). Learning new word meanings from story reading: The benefit of immediate testing. PeerJ, 9, e11693. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.11693
  • James, E., Gaskell, M. G., & Henderson, L. M. (2019). Offline consolidation supersedes prior knowledge benefits in children's (but not adults’) word learning. Developmental Science, 22(3), e12776. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12776
  • James, E., Gaskell, M. G., & Henderson, L. M. (2020a). Sleep-dependent consolidation in children with comprehension and vocabulary weaknesses: It’ll be alright on the night? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(10), 1104–1115. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13253
  • James, E., Gaskell, M. G., Pearce, R., Korell, C., Dean, C., & Henderson, L.-M. (2020b). The role of prior lexical knowledge in children's and adults’ incidental word learning from illustrated stories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(11), 1856–1869. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vm5ad
  • James, E., Gaskell, M. G., Weighall, A., & Henderson, L. (2017). Consolidation of vocabulary during sleep: The rich get richer? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.054
  • Kumaran, D., Hassabis, D., & McClelland, J. L. (2016). What learning systems do intelligent agents need? Complementary learning systems theory updated. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(7), 512–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.004
  • Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 978–990. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  • Lampe, L. F., Hameau, S., & Nickels, L. (2022). Semantic variables both help and hinder word production: Behavioral evidence from picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48(1), 72–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001050
  • Mak, M. H., Hsiao, Y., & Nation, K. (2021). Anchoring and contextual variation in the early stages of incidental word learning during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 118, 104203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104203
  • Mak, M. H., & Twitchell, H. (2020). Evidence for preferential attachment: Words that are more well connected in semantic networks are better at acquiring new links in paired-associate learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(5), 1059–1069. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01773-0
  • Marian, V., Bartolotti, J., Chabal, S., & Shook, A. (2012). Clearpond: Cross-linguistic easy-access resource for phonological and orthographic neighborhood densities. PloS one, 7(8), e43230. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043230
  • McClelland, J. L. (2013). Incorporating rapid neocortical learning of new schema-consistent information into complementary learning systems theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1190. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033812
  • McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & Lampinen, A. K. (2020). Integration of new information in memory: New insights from a complementary learning systems perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1799), 20190637. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0637
  • McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O'Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102(3), 419. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419
  • McGregor, K. K., Licandro, U., Arenas, R., Eden, N., Stiles, D., Bean, A., & Walker, E. (2013). Why words are hard for adults with developmental language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(6), 1845–1856. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0233)
  • McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 547–559. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192726
  • Mirman, D., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Attractor dynamics and semantic neighborhood density: Processing is slowed by near neighbors and speeded by distant neighbors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(1), 65. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.65
  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of south florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 402–407. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195588
  • Peiffer, A., Brichet, M., De Tiège, X., Peigneux, P., & Urbain, C. (2020). Re-epithelialization and immune cell behaviour in an ex vivo human skin model. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56847-4
  • Perry, L. K., Axelsson, E. L., & Horst, J. S. (2016). Learning what to remember: Vocabulary knowledge and children's memory for object names and features. Infant and Child Development, 25(4), 247–258. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1933
  • Pexman, P. M. (2020). How does meaning come to mind? Four broad principles of semantic processing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 74(4), 275–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000235
  • Pexman, P. M., Hargreaves, I. S., Siakaluk, P. D., Bodner, G. E., & Pope, J. (2008). There are many ways to be rich: Effects of three measures of semantic richness on visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 161–167. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.1.161
  • Pexman, P. M., & Yap, M. J. (2018). Individual differences in semantic processing: Insights from the calgary semantic decision project. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(7), 1091. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000499
  • Qualtrics (2014). Qualtrics [software]. Provo, UT, USA: Qualtrics.
  • Rabovsky, M., Schad, D. J., & Rahman, R. A. (2016). Language production is facilitated by semantic richness but inhibited by semantic density: Evidence from picture naming. Cognition, 146, 240–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.016
  • R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/.
  • Rezlescu, C. (2015). Testable: The web-based platform for creating, running and sharing behavioural experiments. Association for Psychological Science Teaching institute conference, New York, 21–24 May.
  • Smalle, E. H., Page, M. P., Duyck, W., Edwards, M., & Szmalec, A. (2018). Children retain implicitly learned phonological sequences better than adults: A longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12634. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12634
  • Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(6), 1321–1337. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/103)
  • Storkel, H. L. (2009). Developmental differences in the effects of phonological, lexical and semantic variables on word learning by infants. Journal of Child Language, 36(02), 291–321. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090800891X
  • Storkel, H. L., & Adlof, S. M. (2009). The effect of semantic set size on word learning by preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 306–320. http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/data/Journals/JSLHR/929279/jslhr_52_2_306.pdf. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0175)
  • Storkel, H. L., Armbrüster, J., & Hogan, T. P. (2006). Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1175–1192. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/085)
  • Storkel, H. L., Bontempo, D. E., Aschenbrenner, A. J., Maekawa, J., & Lee, S.-Y. (2013). The effect of incremental changes in phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on word learning by preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(5), 1689–1700. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0245)
  • Tamminen, J., Davis, M. H., Merkx, M., & Rastle, K. (2012). The role of memory consolidation in generalisation of new linguistic information. Cognition, 125(1), 107–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.014
  • Tamminen, J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2013). Novel word integration in the mental lexicon: Evidence from unmasked and masked semantic priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 1001–1025. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.724694
  • Tamminen, J., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Lewis, P. A. (2013). The role of sleep spindles and slow-wave activity in integrating new information in semantic memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(39), 15376–15381. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5093-12.2013
  • Tamminen, J., Payne, J. D., Stickgold, R., Wamsley, E. J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2010). Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(43), 14356–14360. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3028-10.2010
  • Tham, E. K., Lindsay, S., & Gaskell, M. G. (2015). Markers of automaticity in sleep-associated consolidation of novel words. Neuropsychologia, 71, 146–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.025
  • Tse, D., Langston, R. F., Kakeyama, M., Bethus, I., Spooner, P. A., Wood, E. R., Witter, M. P., & Morris, R. G. (2007). Schemas and memory consolidation. Science, 316(5821), 76–82. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135935
  • van der Kleij, S. W., Rispens, J. E., & Scheper, A. R. (2016). The effect of phonotactic probability and neighbourhood density on pseudoword learning in 6- and 7-year-old children. First Language, 36(2), 93–108. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723715626064
  • Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of intelligence. Pearson.
  • Wickham, H. (2009). Ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer Science & Business Media.
  • Wilhelm, I., Prehn-Kristensen, A., & Born, J. (2012). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation – what can be learnt from children? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(7), 1718–1728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.03.002
  • Wilhelm, I., Rose, M., Imhof, K. I., Rasch, B., Büchel, C., & Born, J. (2013). The sleeping child outplays the adult's capacity to convert implicit into explicit knowledge. Nature Neuroscience, 16(4), 391-393. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3343